바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Korean Journal of School Psychology

The Relations between Children's Peer Status and Mindreading

Abstract

This research investigates the relations between children's peer status and mind reading abilities in preadolescence. Based on sociometric methods(Coie & Dodge, 1983), 241 12-year-olds(1l5 girls; 126 boys; mean age 12-2) were divided into 5 groups: the popular, controversial, average, neglected and the rejected group. The mind reading abilities were measured by two kinds of tasks: the thoughts reading tasks and the emotions reading tasks. The theory of mind tasks such as Faux pas, white lie, and irony were used as the thoughts reading tasks. Reading the emotional states through eyes or facial expressions was used as the emotions reading tasks. Girls performed significantly better than boys on the thoughts reading and the emotions reading tasks. In the girls group, the thoughts reading and emotions reading scores differed among five peer status, but in the boys group the thoughts reading scores were not differed. However the mind reading scores which were the means of the thoughts reading and the emotions reading differed among peer status in both the girls and boys groups. More specifically, the mind reading performances of the popular group were higher than those of the rejected group. Overall, the results indicated that the popular children's mind reading abilities were higher than those of the rejected children. In addition, girls' mind reading abilities were higher than those boys. These findings suggest that the midreading abilities are important for the peer relations and higher in girls than in boys. These results support the Baron-Cohen(2003)'s arguments that girls have a better understanding of other people's mental states.

keywords
마음읽기, 생각읽기, 정서읽기, 또래지위, 사회측정기법, mind reading, thoughts reading, emotions reading, peer status, preadolescence

Reference

1.

구재선, 김혜리, 김경미, 양혜영, 한미영, 정명숙, 박상규 (2008). 교도소 재소자의 마음 이론과 공감. 한국 심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 22(4), 43-57.

2.

구재선, 김혜리, 양혜영, 김경미, 정명숙, 이수미, 최현옥 (2008). 중학생의 마음이해 능력과 사회적 상호작용. 한국 심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 22(2), 17-33.

3.

김윤경, 이옥경 (2001). 아동과 청소년의 또래 관계에 따른 심리사회적 적응과 행동특성. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 14(2), 65-82.

4.

김혜리, 구재선, 김경미, 양혜영, 정명숙, 최현옥, 이수미 (2007). 학령기와 그 이후의 마음이해 발달: 애매한 상황에서의 마음읽기 능력. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 20(1), 19-47.

5.

김혜리, 이숙희 (2005). 인기있는 아동은 마음 읽기를 잘하나?. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 18(1), 1-18.

6.

신유림 (2005). 아동의 틀린 믿음 및 정서이해 능력과 인기도 및 친구관계의 관련성. 대한가정학회지, 43(8), 13-23.

7.

양혜영, 김혜리, 김경미, 구재선, 박은혜, 정명숙 (2008). 초등학생의 마음이해와 사회적 능력의 관계. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 21(1), 31-47

8.

조경자, 박수진, 송인혜, 김혜리 (2007). 정서상태와 얼굴표정 간의 연결 능력의 발달. 한국감성과학회, 10(1), 127-138

9.

Badenes, L., Estevan, R., & Bacete, F. (2000). Theory of mind and peer rejection at school. Social Development, 9(3), 271-283.

10.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). The mindreading system: New directions for research. Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 724-750.

11.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

12.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2003) The essential difference: The truth about the male and female brain. Perseus Publishing.

13.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The Empathizing system: A revision of the 1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

14.

Baron-Cohen, S., O'Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 409-418.

15.

Baron-Cohen, S., Riviere, A., Cross, P., Fukushima, M., Bryant, C., Sotillo, M., Hadwin, J., & French, D. (1996). Reading the mind in the face: A cross cultural and developmental study. Visual Cognition, 3(1), 39-59.

16.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241-251.

17.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes?” Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4(3), 311-331.

18.

Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of Mind in Preadolescence: Relations Between Social Understanding and Social Competence. Social Development, 8(2), 237-255

19.

Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Bukowski, W. M., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2001). Developmental profiles of peer social preference over the course of elementary school: Associations with trajectories of externalizing and internalizing behavior. Developmental psychology, 37(3), 308-320

20.

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimension and types of social status: Across age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 557-570.

21.

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1983). A behavioral analysis of energing social status in boy's groups. Child Development, 54, 1400-1461

22.

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). Peer group behavior and social status. In S. R. Asher & J. D. Coie(Eds.), Peer Rejection in Childhood (pp.17-59). New York: Cambridge University Press.

23.

Dockett, S., Degotardi, S., Szarkowicz, D., & Rovers, F. (1996). Young children's peer popularity and theories of mind. Paper presented at the Educational Research Association/ Australian Association for research in Education, Singapore.

24.

Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral anteceents of peer social status. Child development, 54, 1356-1399.

25.

Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Pettit, G. T., & Price, J. M.(1990). Peer status and aggression in boy's groups: Developmental and contextual analyses. Child Development, 61, 1289-1309.

26.

Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg H. (2003) The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science. 6, 346-359.

27.

Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129-154.

28.

Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843-855.

29.

Huges, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590-619.

30.

Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive versus withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 879-96.

31.

Lease, A. M., Kennedy, C. A., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002) Children's Social Constructions of popularity. Social Development, 11(1), 87-109

32.

Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Mindreading and moral awareness in popular and rejected preschoolers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 205-224.

33.

Puttallaz, M., & Gottman, J. M. (1981). Social skill and social acceptance. In The Development of Children's Friendship, ed. by S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman, New York: Cambridge University Press.

34.

Repacholi, B., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Individual differences in theory of mind: Implication for typacal and atypical development(pp.67-97). New York: Psychology Press.

35.

Slaughter, V., Denis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 545-564.

36.

Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

37.

Whiten, A., & Perner, J. (1991). Fundamental issues in the multidisciplinary study of mindreading. in A. Whiten(Ed.), Natural theories of mind. Oxford: Bl.

Korean Journal of School Psychology